In my recent BBC article and my blog posts about emoji, I have tried to explain how emoji are not an emerging language, but that they do serve important functions that resemble other limited communicative systems.
Having now poked around online quite a bit looking at what people say about emoji, I’m particularly struck by the repetition of a few myths. Since these misunderstandings creep up all over the place, I wanted to address them here…
1. Emoji are not like hieroglyphics
First off, many people have compared emoji to Egyptian hieroglyphics, either saying that they work exactly the same and/or that emoji are a “modern hieroglyphics.”
This is simply not true, mostly because hieroglyphics were a full blown writing system where each sign had a mapping to sound. Hieroglyphics are not “symbol systems” made up of pictures. To me, this seems like the kind of misperception that people who are only used to an alphabet have about other writing systems: “if each sign isn’t a sound like a letter, it must be just about meanings!”
There are actually several ways that hieroglyphics operated as a writing system. Some signs did indeed mean what they represented. For example, the sign for “owl” looked like an owl, and was pronounced “m”:
However, the use of “rebus” signs meant that those signs could also be used without that iconic meaning, and only would be used for their sound value (i.e., that owl sign would be used for many words using the sound “m,” but not for its meaning of “owl”).
From there, these both of these types of signs could be combined into compound signs. For example, this combination takes the rebus of owl (using just the sound “m”) and the sign for ear (using its meaning, but not pronunciation) for the word “to hear”:
This type of compound used signs both for their meaning value and for their sound value. There are no compounds made up of two signs that just contribute to meaning—they always have some sound-based sign present. Hieroglyphics also sometimes use fairly abstract representations, and purely sound-based signs which vary based on the number of consonants they represent.
In sum, unlike the purely imagistic meanings found in emoji, hieroglyphics are a fully functioning writing system that is intrinsically tied to the Egyptian language. This is totally different from emoji in context also because the imagistic emoji accompany a separate writings system (for English speakers, the alphabet). In the case of hieroglyphics, they are the writing system.
I’ll note also, these same things apply to Chinese characters. Though they work a little different than hieroglyphics, the same basic principles apply: it’s a writing system tied to the sounds of languages, not a series of pictures that only have imagistic meaning.
2. There is no such thing as a universal language
I have seen many people exhort that one of the exciting things about emoji is their quality of transcending spoken languages to be a “universal language.” This is also hogwash, for many reasons. No language is universal, whether verbal, signed, or visual. Here are several reasons why images (including emoji) are not, and cannot be, universal:
How they convey meaning
Just because images may be iconic—they look like what they represent—does not mean that they are culturally universal. Even simple things like the way people dress does not translate across cultures, not to mention variables in facial expressions or, even more dramatic, fully conventionalized meanings like giant sweat drops to convey anxiety. Note that, since they were created in Japan originally, many emoji are already culturally specific in ways that do not translate well outside Japan.
This is not to mention the limitations of emoji that I discussed in my BBC article, such as that they rely on a non-producible vocabulary that does not allow the easy creation of new signs, and their sequence maintain a simple system characteristic of impoverished grammar. In other words, they are highly limited in what they can express, even as a graphic system.
Cultural exposure
We also know that images are not universal because a host of studies have shown that people who do not have cultural exposure to images often have difficulty understanding the meanings of images. Such deficits were investigated prevalently in the 1970s and 1980s under the umbrella of “visual literacy.” Here’s how I summarized one such study examining individuals from Nepal from Fussell & Haaland (1978):
As described in the paper, the individuals tested “had significant deficits understanding many aspects of single images, even images of faces with simple emotions in cartoony styles (happy – 33%, sad – 60%). They had even more difficultly related to actions (only 3% understood an image trying to convey information about drinking boiled water). Some respondents had radically different interpretations of images. For example, a fairly simple cartoony image of a pregnant woman was interpretted as a woman by 75%, but 11% thought it was a man, and others responded that it was a cow, rabbit, bird, or other varied interpretations. They also had difficulty understanding when images cut off parts of individuals by the framing of a panel (such as images of only hands holding different ingredients).”
Such findings are not rare. For example, Research into Illustration by Evelyn Goldsmith summarizes several studies along these lines. Bottom line: Understanding drawings requires exposure to a graphic system, just like learning a language.
There is not just one visual language
Most discussion of the universality of images focuses on how they are comprehended. But, this overlooks the fact that someone also had to create those images, and images vary widely in their patterns across the world.
That is, as I argue in my book, there is not just one visual language, but rather there are many visual languages in the world too. There’s a reason why the “style” of American superhero comics differs from Japanese manga or French band desinee or instruction manuals, etc. Drawing “styles” reflect the patterns of graphic information stored in the minds of those who create them. These patterns vary across the world, both within and between cultures.
This happens because people are different and brains are not perfect. There will always be variation and change within what is perceived to be a coherent system. This is in part because any given language is actually a socio-cultural label applied to the system(s) used by individual people. There is no “English” floating out in the ether to which we all link up. Rather, “English” is created by the similarities of patterning between the languages spoken by many people.
Indeed, though many who speak “English” can communicate in mutually intelligible ways, there are hundreds of sub-varieties of “English” with variations that range from subtle (slight accents) to dramatic (changing vocabulary and grammar), both across geographic, cultural, and generational dimensions.
Similarly, even if there was to be a universal language—be it spoken or visual—sub-varieties would emerge based on who is using the system and how they do it. Just because images are typically iconic does not mean that they are transparent and outside of cognitive/cultural patterns.
Emoji in part exemplify this facade that a language is external to the patterns in people’s minds, since the vocabulary is provided by tech companies and does not directly emerge from people’s creations. Someone (ICANN) decides which emoji can be used, and then makes them available. This is the opposite of how actual languages work, as manifestations of similarities between cognitive structures across speakers.
In sum, drawings are not universal because drawings differ based on the cultural “visual languages” that result from people using different patterns across the world.
Comments